Relationship between Spatial Thinking and Puzzle Games of Elementary School Students

Author :  

Year-Number: 2018-Volume 10, Issue 1
Language : null
Konu : null

Abstract

The present research aims to examine the relationship between the spatial thinking skills of fourth-grade primary school students and their abilities pertaining to puzzle games. Spatial thinking refers to the mental manipulation of object sand thus the ability to move physically over space, time, and distance. The term also connotes a range of mental skills including intelligence with regard to the associations that stem from objects, their shapes, their paths, and how they move over distance. Spatial thinking skills are thought to be both innate and acquired. Humans are born with a fundamental cognition of space and distance and as children grow and experience their environment, they hone their spatial thinking capabilities. In this context, it is thought that the spatial thinking skills of children who frequently play with visual games and puzzles, such as locating patterns, finding symmetry, matching shapes or objects, identifying, combining and breaking up shapes, and working on jigsaws, are much more evolved. Based on this hypothesis, the correlational method was used in the study. In total, 117 students from a state school in Adana participated. Two measuring instruments were used. A geometry test was used to measure the spatial visualization skills of the students in two-dimensional geometry and another test was used to measure their proficiency in the puzzle games. The results revealed a positive and significant relationship between the puzzle test and the spatial visualization tests.

Keywords

Abstract

The present research aims to examine the relationship between the spatial thinking skills of fourth-grade primary school students and their abilities pertaining to puzzle games. Spatial thinking refers to the mental manipulation of object sand thus the ability to move physically over space, time, and distance. The term also connotes a range of mental skills including intelligence with regard to the associations that stem from objects, their shapes, their paths, and how they move over distance. Spatial thinking skills are thought to be both innate and acquired. Humans are born with a fundamental cognition of space and distance and as children grow and experience their environment, they hone their spatial thinking capabilities. In this context, it is thought that the spatial thinking skills of children who frequently play with visual games and puzzles, such as locating patterns, finding symmetry, matching shapes or objects, identifying, combining and breaking up shapes, and working on jigsaws, are much more evolved. Based on this hypothesis, the correlational method was used in the study. In total, 117 students from a state school in Adana participated. Two measuring instruments were used. A geometry test was used to measure the spatial visualization skills of the students in two-dimensional geometry and another test was used to measure their proficiency in the puzzle games. The results revealed a positive and significant relationship between the puzzle test and the spatial visualization tests.

Keywords


  • Büyüköztürk, Ş., Kılıç Çakmak, E., Akgün, Ö.E., Karadeniz, Ş. ve Demirel, F. (2012). Bilimsel araştırma yöntemleri. Ankara: PegemA Yayıncılık.

  • Büyüköztürk, Ş. (2011). Sosyal bilimler için veri analizi el kitabı: İstatistik, araştırma deseni, SPSS uygulamaları ve yorum. Ankara: PegemA Yayıncılık.

  • Burrill, G. (Ed.) (2006). NCTM 2006 Yearbook: Thinking and reasoning with data and chance, 309-321, Reston, VA: NCTM.

  • Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., & Hare, T. A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 111-126. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.010

  • Danışman, Ş., & Erginer, E. (2017). The predictive power of fifth graders’ learning styles on their mathematical reasoning and spatial ability. Cogent Education, 4(1), 1-18. doi:10.1080/2331186X.2016.1266830

  • Ehrlich, S. B., Levine, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The importance of gesture in children’s spatial reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 42, 1259–1268. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1259

  • Ergin, U. A. S., & Türnüklü, E. (2015). Ortaokul öğrencilerinin cisim imgelerinin incelenmesi: Geometrik ve uzamsal düşünme ile ilişkiler. Eğitim ve Öğretim Aras ̧tırmaları Dergisi, 4(2), 188-199.

  • Ferrara, K., Hirsh‐Pasek, K., Newcombe, N. S., Golinkoff, R. M., & Lam, W. S. (2011). Block talk: Spatial language during block play. Mind, Brain, and Education, 5(3), 143-151. doi: 10.1111/j.1751228X.2011.01122.x

  • Gentner, D., Özyürek, A., Gürcanli, Ö., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2013). Spatial language facilitates spatial cognition: Evidence from children who lack language input. Cognition, 127(3), 318-330. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.01.003

  • Hacıömeroğlu, G., & Hacıömeroğlu, E. S. (2017). Cinsiyet, uzamsal beceri, mantıksal düşünme becerisi ve çözüm tercihleri arasındaki ilişkinin incelenmesi. Adıyaman University Journal of Educational Sciences, 7(1), 116-131. doi: 10.17984/adyuebd.310833

  • İrioğlu, Z., & Ertekin, E. (2011). İlköğretim ikinci kademe öğrencilerinin zihinsel döndürme becerilerinin bazı değişkenler açısından incelenmesi. Journal of Educational and Instructional Studies in the World, 2(1), 7588.

  • Jirout, J. J., & Newcombe, N. S. (2015). Building blocks for developing spatial skills: evidence from a large, representative US sample. Psychological Science, 26(3), 302-310. doi: 10.1177/0956797614563338

  • Kösa, T. (2011). Ortaöğretim öğrencilerinin uzamsal yeteneklerinin incelenmesi. Yayınlanmamış doktora tezi, Karadeniz Teknik Üniversitesi Eğitim Bilimleri Enstitüsü, Trabzon.

  • Lawton, C. A. (2010). Gender, spatial abilities, and way finding. In J. C. Chrisler & D. R. Mc Creary (Eds.), Handbook of gender research in psychology, (pp.317-341). Springer New York.

  • Levine, S. C., Ratliff, K. R., Huttenlocher, J., & Cannon, J. (2012). Early puzzle play: a predictor of preschoolers' spatial transformation skill. Developmental Psychology, 48(2), 530-542. doi: 10.1037/a0025913

  • Levine, S. C., Huttenlocher, J., Taylor, A., & Langrock, A. (1999). Early sex differences in spatial skill. Developmental Psychology, 35, 940–949. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.940

  • Milli Eğitim Bakanlığı (MEB). (2016). STEM Eğitimi raporu. Ankara: Milli Eğitim Bakanlığı Yenilik ve Eğitim Teknolojileri Genel Müdürlüğü (YEĞİTEK)

  • National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.

  • National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2006). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

  • Olkun, S. (2003b). Comparing computer versus concrete manipulatives in learning 2D geometry. Journal of Computers in Mathematics and Science Teaching, 22(1), 43-56.

  • Olkun, S. & Altun, A. (2003). İlköğretim öğrencilerinin bilgisayar deneyimleri ile uzamsal düşünme ve geometri başarıları arasındaki ilişki. The Turkish Online Journal of Educational Technology, 2(4), 86-91.

  • Pruden, S. M., Levine, S. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (2011). Children’s spatial thinking: does talk about the spatial world matter?. Developmental science, 14(6), 1417-1430. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01088.x

  • Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E.A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). Spatial anxiety relates to spatial abilities as a function of working memory in children, The Quarterly Journal of Experımental Psychology, 65(3), 474487, doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-9996-2

  • Roorda, J. (1994). Visual perception, spatial visualization and engineering drawing. Engineering Design GraphicsJournal 58(2), 12-21.

  • Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2009). Playing linear number board games—but not circular ones—improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 545-560. doi: 10.1037/a0014239

  • Turgut, M., & Yılmaz, S. (2012). Relationships among preservice primary mathematics teachers' gender, academic success and spatial ability. International Journal of Instruction, 5(2), 5-20.

  • Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 352–402. doi:10.1037/a0028446

  • Verdine, B. N., Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh‐Pasek, K., Newcombe, N. S., Filipowicz, A. T., & Chang, A. (2014). Deconstructing building blocks: Preschoolers' spatial assembly performance relates to early mathematical skills. Child development, 85(3), 1062-1076. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12165

  • Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM domains: Aligning over fifty years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 817–835. doi: 10.1037/a0016127

  • Whyte, J. C., & Bull, R. (2008). Number games, magnitude representation, and basic number skills in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 44(2), 588-596. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.588

  • Yıldırım, B., & Altun, Y. (2015). STEM eğitim ve mühendislik uygulamalarının fen bilgisi laboratuar dersindeki etkilerinin incelenmesi. El-Cezeri Journal of Science and Engineering, 2(2), 28-40.

  • Zhang, D., Ding, Y., Stegall, J. & Mo, L. (2012). The effect of visual‐chunking‐representation accommodation on geometry testing for students with math disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 27(4), 167-177. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2012.00364.x

                                                                                                                                                                                                        
  • Article Statistics