Chronotype And Time Of Day Do Not Influence Mathematical Achievement İn Standardised Tests, But Impact On Affect – Results From A Field Experiment

Author :  

Year-Number: 2016-Volume 8, Issue 5
Language : null
Konu : null

Abstract

Morning chronotypes have a higher academic achievement. In addition, laboratory studies showed synchrony effects between chronotype and test scores with morning people having a higher test score when tested in the morning and that evening people having higher test score in the afternoon. Field experiments are scarce. We developed a cross-over design and applied two different tests at two different time points during school days. A total of 90 (47 boys, 43 girls) pupils from four different 9th grade classrooms. We used the Composite Scale of Morningness, the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) and two parallelized mathematics tests that had the same content and the same scoring (test A, test B). There was no general effect of time of day (test scores: morning 12.21 ± 2.41; noon: 11.89 ± 2.14; t=1.105; p=0.272). There were no correlations between test scores and the continuous CSM scores, nor were there differences between chronotype classifications, and thus, no synchrony effect. Positive Affect increased between morning and noon in all three chronotypes. Morning types had a higher positive affect than evening types during the morning (F=7.974, p=0.001) and at noon (F= 4.561, p=0.013; Figure 2). Negative Affect remained stable in morning types and evening types but changed significantly in neither types. Negative Affect was not different between the chronotypes (morning: F=0.063, p=0.965; noon: F=0.756, p=0.473). Negative Affect was positively related to test scores in mathematics but only in the morning (morning test: r=0.25, p=0.02; noon test: r=-0.032, p=0.775). We strongly emphasize that there should be more experimental field studies in schools to shed light on this topic before making suggestions for teaching and learning

Keywords

Abstract

Morning chronotypes have a higher academic achievement. In addition, laboratory studies showed synchrony effects between chronotype and test scores with morning people having a higher test score when tested in the morning and that evening people having higher test score in the afternoon. Field experiments are scarce. We developed a cross-over design and applied two different tests at two different time points during school days. A total of 90 (47 boys, 43 girls) pupils from four different 9th grade classrooms. We used the Composite Scale of Morningness, the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) and two parallelized mathematics tests that had the same content and the same scoring (test A, test B). There was no general effect of time of day (test scores: morning 12.21 ± 2.41; noon: 11.89 ± 2.14; t=1.105; p=0.272). There were no correlations between test scores and the continuous CSM scores, nor were there differences between chronotype classifications, and thus, no synchrony effect. Positive Affect increased between morning and noon in all three chronotypes. Morning types had a higher positive affect than evening types during the morning (F=7.974, p=0.001) and at noon (F= 4.561, p=0.013; Figure 2). Negative Affect remained stable in morning types and evening types but changed significantly in neither types. Negative Affect was not different between the chronotypes (morning: F=0.063, p=0.965; noon: F=0.756, p=0.473). Negative Affect was positively related to test scores in mathematics but only in the morning (morning test: r=0.25, p=0.02; noon test: r=-0.032, p=0.775). We strongly emphasize that there should be more experimental field studies in schools to shed light on this topic before making suggestions for teaching and learning

Keywords


  • Arbabi, T., Vollmer, C., Dörfler, T., & Randler, C. (2015). The influence of chronotype, intelligence, conscientiousness, and motivation on academic achievement in primary school. Chronobiology International, 32, 349-357. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2014.980508.

  • Díaz-Morales, J. F., & Escribano, C. (2013). Predicting school achievement: The role of inductive reasoning, sleep length and morningness–eveningness. Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 106-111.

  • Díaz-Morales, J., Escribano, C., & Jankowski, K. (2015). Chronotype and time-of-day effects on mood during school day. Chronobiology International, 32(1), 37-42.

  • Escribano, C., & Díaz-Morales, J. F. (2016). Are achievement goals different among morning and evening – type adolescents? Personality and Individual Differences, 88, 57-61. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.032.

  • Escribano, C., & Díaz-Morales, J. (2014). Daily fluctuations in attention at school considering starting time and chronotype: an exploratory study. Chronobiology International, 316), 761-769.

  • Gläser-Zikuda, M., Fuß, S., Laukenmann, M., Metz, K., & Randler, C. (2005). Promoting students' emotions and achievement–Instructional design and evaluation of the ECOLE-approach. Learning and Instruction, 15(5), 481-495.

  • Goldstein, D., Hahn, C.S., Hasher, L., Wiprzycka, U.J., & Zelazo, P.D. (2007). Time of day, intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in morning versus evening type adolescents: Is there a synchrony effect? Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 431-440.

  • Hahn, C., Cowell, J. M., Wiprzycka, U. J., Goldstein, D., Ralph, M., Hasher, L., & Zelazo, P. D. (2012). Circadian rhythms in executive function during the transition to adolescence: the effect of synchrony between chronotype and time of day. Developmental Science, 15(3), 408-416.

  • Heller, K., & Hany, E. (2002). Standartisierte Schulleistungsmessungen. In F. Weinert, Leistungsmessungen in Schulen (S. 87-101). Weinheim und Basel: Beltz Verlag.

  • Itzek-Greulich, H., Randler, C., & Vollmer, C. (2016). The interaction of chronotype and time of day in a science course: Adolescent evening types learn more and are more motivated in the afternoon. Learning and Individual Differences, 51, 189-198.

  • Krohne, H., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C., & Tausch, A. (1996). Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Version der "Positive and Negative Affect Schedule" (PANAS). Diagnostica, 2, 139-156.

  • Öztürk, E. (2014). The effect of circadian rhythm on elementary students' reading comprehension. Biological Rhythm Research, 45 (6), 861-868.

  • Pant, H., Stanat, P., Schroeders, U., Roppelt, A., Siegle, T., & Pöhlmann, C. (2013). IQB-Ländervergleich 2012 - Mathematische und naturwissenschaftliche Kompetenzen am Ende der Sekundarstufe 1. Münster/New York/München/Berlin: Waxmann Verlag.

  • Preckel, F., Lipnevich, A. A., Schneider, S., & Roberts, R. D. (2011). Chronotype, cognitive abilities, and academic achievement: A meta-analytic investigation. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 483-492. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.003.

  • Preckel, F., Lipnevich, A. A., Boehme, K., Brandner, L., Georgi, K., Könen, T., Mursin, K., & Roberts, R. D. (2013). Morningness-eveningness and educational outcomes: The lark has an advantage over the owl at high school. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 114-134. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2011.02059.x.

  • Quinn, G., & Keough, M. (2002). Experimental Design and Data Analysis for Biologists. Cambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Kapstadt, Singapur, Sao Paulo: Cambridge University Verlag.

  • Rahafar, A., Maghsudloo, M., Farhangnia, S., Vollmer, C., & Randler, C. (2015). The role of chronotype, gender, test anxiety, and conscientiousness in academic achievement of high school students. Chronobiology International, 33, 1-9. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2015.1107084.

  • Randler, C., & Frech, D. (2006). Correlation between morningness-eveningness and final school leaving exams. Biological Rhythm Research, 37, 233-239. doi: 10.1080/09291010600645780.

  • Randler, C. & D. Frech (2009). Young people's time of day preferences affect their school performance. Journal of Youth Studies, 12, 653-667.

  • Randler, C. (2011). Age and gender differences in morningness-eveningness during adolescence. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 172, 302-308.

  • Randler, C., Rahafar, A., Arbabi, T., & Bretschneider, R. (2014). Affective state of school pupils during their first lesson of the day – effect of morningness-eveningness. Mind, Brain & Education, 8, 214-219.

  • Randler, C. & Weber, V. (2015). Positive and negative affect during the school day and its relationship to morningness-eveningness. Biological Rhythm Research, 46, 683-690.

  • Rossi, V., Pourtois, G. (2012). Transient state-dependent fluctuations in anxiety measured using STAI, POMS, PANAS or VAS: a comparative review. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 25, 603–645.

  • Ruffing, S., Hahn, E., Spinath, F. M., Brünken, R., & Karbach, J. (2015). Predicting students’ learning strategies: The contribution of chronotype over personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 85, 199–204.

  • Thompson, E. R. 2007. Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 227–242.

  • Tonetti, L., Adan, A., Di Milia, L., Randler, C., & Natale, V. (2015). Measures of circadian preference in childhood and adolescence: A review. European Psychiatry, 30(5), 576-582.

  • Tonetti, L., Natale, V., & Randler, C. (2015). Association between circadian preference and academic achievement: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Chronobiology International, 32(6), 792-801.

  • Vollmer, C., Pötsch, F., & Randler, C. (2013). Morningness is associated with better gradings and higher attention in class. Learning and Individual Differences, 27, 167-173.

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063-1070.

                                                                                                                                                                                                        
  • Article Statistics