Diurnal Variations And Weekly Pattern Of Cognitive Performances İn Tunisian Children

Author :  

Year-Number: 2016-Volume 8, Issue 5
Language : null
Konu : null

Abstract

The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of time of day and day of test on cognitive performances in Tunisian children in terms of constant attention (CA) and choice reaction time (RT). The protocol involved 132 children who performed two cognitive tasks, a choice reaction-test, and the barrage test (measuring the CA), at 09:00, 11:00, 14:00 and 16:00 h, over six days of the week (Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday, Friday and Saturday). Resting oral temperature was measured at the beginning of each test session. The results showed significant time-of-day effects with both tests and also core temperature with best performances for the choice RT and highest core temperatures at 16:00 h, and best performances for the CA observed at 09:00 h. In addition, our results displayed significant day of test effect on both cognitive performances with best scores in the middle (Tuesday, Wednesday and Thursday) of the week and worst scores in the beginning (Monday) and the end of the week (Friday and Saturday). However, the profiles of the rhythms of core temperatures and cognitive performances were not the same. Therefore, school activities that involve new learning and require vigilance and better cognitive performances should be administered in the middle of the week. Otherwise, tasks which require concentration should be programmed in the morning, while tasks and brief applications which require speed should be administered in the afternoon.

Keywords

Abstract

The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of time of day and day of test on cognitive performances in Tunisian children in terms of constant attention (CA) and choice reaction time (RT). The protocol involved 132 children who performed two cognitive tasks, a choice reaction-test, and the barrage test (measuring the CA), at 09:00, 11:00, 14:00 and 16:00 h, over six days of the week (Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday, Friday and Saturday). Resting oral temperature was measured at the beginning of each test session. The results showed significant time-of-day effects with both tests and also core temperature with best performances for the choice RT and highest core temperatures at 16:00 h, and best performances for the CA observed at 09:00 h. In addition, our results displayed significant day of test effect on both cognitive performances with best scores in the middle (Tuesday, Wednesday and Thursday) of the week and worst scores in the beginning (Monday) and the end of the week (Friday and Saturday). However, the profiles of the rhythms of core temperatures and cognitive performances were not the same. Therefore, school activities that involve new learning and require vigilance and better cognitive performances should be administered in the middle of the week. Otherwise, tasks which require concentration should be programmed in the morning, while tasks and brief applications which require speed should be administered in the afternoon.

Keywords


  • Barthe, B. (2004). L’analyse de l’activité de travail en poste de nuit : bilan de 25 ans de recherches et perspective. Le travail humain. 1,112.

  • Beau, J., Carlier, M., Duyme, M0, Capron, C., Perez-Diaz, F. (1999). Procedure to extract a weekly pattern of performance of human reaction time. Percept Mot Skills, 88, 469-483.

  • Brand, G. (1996). Social synchronizers and infradian variations of reaction time. C R Seances Soc Biol Fil, 190, 487-496

  • Buysse, D.J., Reynolds III, C.F., Monk, T.H., Berman, S.R., & Kupfer, D.J. (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 28(2), 193213.

  • Carskadon, M.A., Vieira, C., & Acebo, C. (1993). Association between puberty and delayed phase preference. Sleep, 16, 258-262.

  • Charifi, M. (1994). Chronopsychologie scolaire. Etude comparative des variations journalières et hebdomadaires de l’activité intellectuelle d’élèves français et iraniens. Thèse de psychologie, Tours.

  • Collado-Mateo, M. J., Díaz-Morales, J. F., Escribano, C., Delgado, P. (2013). Matutinidad-vespertinidad y ansiedad rasgo en adolescentes. Anales de Psicología, 29(1), 90-93.

  • Delvolve, N., Davila, W. (1996). Effets de la semaine de classe de quatre jours sur l’élève. In : Rythmes scolaires en Europe. Etudes et documents, Enfance, 5 : 400-407.

  • Delvolve, N., Jeunier, B. (1999). Effets de la durée du week-end sur l’état cognitif de l’élève en classe au cours du lundi. Revue Française de Pédagogie, 126 : 111-117.

  • Díaz-Morales, J. F., Dávila de León, M. C., Gutiérrez Sorroche, M. (2007). Validity of the morningness- eveningness scale for children among Spanish adolescents. Chronobiology International, 24(3), 435-447.

  • Díaz-Morales, J. F., Randler, C. (2008). Morningness-eveningness among German and Spanish adolescents 12-18-years-old adolescents. European Psychologist, 13(3), 214-221.

  • Feunteun, P. (2000). Fluctuations journalières de la vigilance en milieu scolaire des élèves de 6 à 11 ans. Effets de l’âge et du niveau scolaire ; effets de synchroniseurs sociaux. Revue de psychologie de l’éducation, 1, 7591.

  • Folkard, S. (1990). Circadian performance rhythms: some practical and theoretical implications. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond, 327, 543-553.

  • Garcia Naveira, A.S., Locatelli Dalimier, L., Ruiz, R. (2015). Morningness-eveningness analysis in high performance young athletes. CCD, 11(10) ,125-134.

  • Goldstein, D., Hahn, C. S., Hasher, L., Wiprzycka, U. J., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Time of day, intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in Morning versus Evening type adolescents: Is there a synchrony effect? Personality and Individual Differences, 42(3), 431-440.

  • Guerin, N., Boulenguiez, S., Reinberg, A., Di costango, G., Guran, P., Touitou, Y. (1993). Weekly changes in psychophysiological variables of 8- to 10-year-old school girls. Chronobiol Int, 10 : 471-479

  • Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., & May, C. P. (2005). It's about time: Circadian rhythms, memory, and aging. In C. Izawa & N. Ohta (Eds.), Human Learning and Memory: Advances in Theory and Application (pp. 199-217). Kansas: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Hasher, L., Lustig, C., & Zacks, R. T. (2007). Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In A. R. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake & J. N. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 227249). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.

  • Janvier, B., Testu, F. (2005). « Développement des fluctuations journalières de l’attention chez les élèves de 4 à 11 ans ». Enfance, 57,155-170.

  • Jarraya, S., Jarraya, M., Souissi, N. (2015). Diurnal variation and weekly pattern on physical performance in Tunisian children. Science & Sports 30, 41—46.

  • Jarraya, S., Jarraya, M., Souissi, N. (2014). Diurnal variations of cognitive performances in Tunisian children. Biol Rhythm Res. 45 (1), 61–67.

  • Kim, S., Duekel, G. L., Hasher, L., & Goldstein, D. (2002). Children's time of day preference: age, gender and ethnic differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 33(7), 1083-1090.

  • Kline, C.E0, Durstine, J.L., Davis, J.M., Moore, T.A., Devlin, T.M., Youngstedt, S.D. (2010). Circadian rhythms of psychomotor, vigilance, mood, and sleepiness in the Ultra-short sleep/wake protocol. Chronobiol Int. 27,161–180.

  • Leconte-Lamber, C. (1994). Des rythmes scolaires à la chronopsychologie de l’éducation : quel intérêt pour les apprentissages à l’école ? In : Santé et apprentissages. La Documentation française: Paris.

  • Locatelli Dalimier, L. (2012). Analisis cronobiopsicologico del futbolista. Evaluacion en Psicologia del deporte. Editorial Academica Espanola. ISBN-10: 3847355511 ISBN-13: 978-3847355519

  • Montagner, H. (2009). Les rythmes majeurs de l'enfant. Sociales. 3(153), 14–20.

  • Montero, P., (2006). Indicateurs de qualité de l'alimentation chez les enfants: l’indice KIDMED. Antropo, 11, 189-197.

  • Nicolas, A., Gauthier, A., Bessot, N., Moussay, S., Davenne, D. (2005). Time-of-day effects on myoelectric and mechanical properties of muscle during maximal and prolonged isokinetic exercise. Chronobiol Int, 22, 997–1011.

  • Reilly, T., Atkinson, G., Edwards, B., Waterhouse, J., Farrelly, K. and Fairhurst, E. (2007) Diurnal variation in temperature, mental and physical performance, and tasks specifically related to football (soccer). Chronobiology International, 24, 507-519.

  • Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., & Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(7), 755-789.

  • Souissi, H., Chtourou, H., Chaouachi, A., Chamari, K., Souissi, N., Amri, M. (2012a). Time-of-day effects on EMG parameters during the Wingate test in boys. J Sports Sci Med. 11, 380–386.

  • Souissi, H., Chtourou, H., Chaouachi, A., Dogui, M., Chamari, K., Souissi, N., Amri, M.. (2012b). The effect of training at a specific time-of-day on the diurnal variations of short-term exercise performances in 10- to 11-year-old boys. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 24, 84–99.

  • Testu, F. (2008). Rythmes de vie et rythmes scolaires: aspects chronobiologiques et chronopsychologiques. Issy-les- Moulineaux: Elsevier Masson. 192.

  • Testu, F. (1994b). Quelques constantes dans les fluctuations journalières et hebdomadaires de l’activité intellectuelle des élèves en Europe. Enfance, 4, 389-400

  • Testu, F. (1994a).Les rythmes scolaires en Europe. Enfance, 4, 367-370

  • Testu, F. (2000). Chronopsychologie et rythmes scolaires. Masson, Paris, 133.

  • Testu, F., Alaphilippe, D., Chasseigne, G., Chèse, M.-T. (1995). Variations journalières de l’activité intellectuelle d’enfants de 10-11 ans en fonction des conditions psychosociologiques de passation d’épreuves, L’Année Psychologique, 247-266.

  • Touitou, Y., Bégué, P. (2010). Aménagement du temps scolaire et santé de l’enfant : Vers un nouvel horaire scolaire. Bull. Acad. Natle Méd, 1, 107-122.

  • Verlaan, P., Cantin, S., & Boivin, M. (2001). L'échelle de développement pubertaire : Équivalence en langue française du « Pubertal Development Scale ». Revue canadienne des sciences et du comportement, 33(3), 143

  • Wickersham, L. (2006). Time-of-Day Preference for Preschool-Aged Children. Annual Review of Undergraduate Research, 5, 259-268.

  • Yoon, C., May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (2000). Aging, circadian arousal patterns, and cognition. In D. C. Park & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognitive aging: A primer (pp. 151-171). Philidelphia, PA, US: Psychology Press.

                                                                                                                                                                                                        
  • Article Statistics